In the last few blogs, we learned a lot about scientific background on touch. To finish that chapter, I will once again list the main ‘tasks’ of the tactile system:
→ touch to communicate emotions – Touch can be used to convey specific emotions such as anger, fear, and contempt. Touch is also used to convey more complicated social messages such as trust, receptivity, affection and nurture, reliance, and affiliation.
→ touch to elicit emotions – It is reasonable to believe that tactile stimulation of various physical locations can trigger a wide range of emotions.
→ touch as a behaviour modulator – Touch has the potential to influence people’s opinions toward people, places, or services, as well as their predisposition to form attachments and (pro-)social activities.
In the following chapters of this blog I would like to learn more about cultural aspect of touch. Since touch is a big part of our social life, I wonder how touch differentiates in cultures. Examining how touch is viewed in different cultures reveals a diverse range of social norms. In some cultures, touch is embraced as a natural and integral part of daily communication, fostering a sense of closeness and connection among individuals. In contrast, other societies may exhibit a more reserved approach to touch, where personal space and boundaries are highly valued.
Every day, we experience social touch in various ways, like greetings (handshakes, hugs, kisses), intimate communication (holding hands, cuddling, back scratching), and corrections (punishment, a pat on the backside). The impact of social touch can be seen in different aspects, from our body’s responses to how we behave in social situations. Even if you don’t consider yourself to be a tactile person, touch is embedded in the social structure of our lives (Halton,2021).
According to ethnographic descriptions, civilizations differ in the amount to which they value physical touch and its appropriateness in various types of social relationships. Different cultures have varying norms for social behavior, including attitudes towards affectionate touch. Nonverbal behaviors are influenced by cultural scripts, and these norms can shape both attitudes and actual behavior. For example, Latino cultures are believed to encourage open expressions of affection, leading to more positive attitudes about affectionate touch among their members. In contrast, mainstream American culture may promote less affectionate touch, resulting in potentially less positive attitudes among its members. Cultural background plays a significant role in shaping people’s views on physical affection (Burleson et al., 2018).
Everyday life observations suggest that humans use similar types of interpersonal touch behavior all over the world to express their affection. However, there were some intriguing differences in the level of intimate touch between the nations. For instance, only around 2% of participants in Italy, Estonia, Romania, Spain, and Mexico said they had not touched their partner at all in the week before the research, compared to almost 16% in the United States, over 20% in Uganda, and 43% in China! Couples in the Czech Republic and Italy, for instance, touch each other more frequently than couples in the United States. Compared to pairs in English, French, or Dutch, those in Greek and Italian had a greater touch predominance. Couples of Latino origin are more likely than Asian couples to engage in affective touch, and Mexican Americans are more likely than European Americans to report feeling comfortable using affective touch in public. It is suggested that cultures can be categorized as high-contact or low-contact based on these observations (Sorokowska et al., 2021).
Since there’s limited research on what influences affectionate touch, especially in terms of cultural factors, we can concentrate on the most well-supported variables. These include (a) cultural aspects like temperature, regional parasite stress, regional conservatism, collectivism, and regional religiosity, and (b) individual factors such as gender, age, personal history with parasites, individual conservatism, individual religiosity, and preferred social distance. To make this blog shorter and not so boring, I will write about some aspects that i found really interesting. The research showed that temperature, e.g. pleasant weather and a warmer climate encourage more frequent social interactions, build trust among people, and make it easier to form social connections. Past research indicates that women tend to use touch more in social interactions, supporting the idea that women typically have broader and denser social networks compared to men. Studies suggest that couples in highly religious countries are less prone to touching each other compared to couples in less religious countries. These findings suggest that religiosity is negatively related to affective touch. These are the aspects that I found the most interesting, if anyone is interested in more, it would be good to look at the research from Sorokowska and others.
Sources:
- Heatley Tejada, A., Dunbar, R.I.M. & Montero, M. Physical Contact and Loneliness: Being Touched Reduces Perceptions of Loneliness. Adaptive Human Behavior and Physiology 6, 292–306 (2020). https://doi.org/10.1007/s40750-020-00138-0
- Halton, M. (2021, March 22). Humans are made to be touched – so what happens when we aren’t? ideas.ted.com. https://ideas.ted.com/we-are-made-to-be-touched-so-what-happens-when-we-arent/
- Burleson, M. H., Roberts, N. A., Coon, D. W., & Soto, J. A. (2018). Perceived cultural acceptability and comfort with affectionate touch. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships, 026540751775000. doi:10.1177/0265407517750005
- Sorokowska, A., Saluja, S., Sorokowski, P., Frąckowiak, T., Karwowski, M., Aavik, T., … Anjum, A. (2021). Affective Interpersonal Touch in Close Relationships: A Cross-Cultural Perspective. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 014616722098837. doi:10.1177/0146167220988373